1994
DOI: 10.1080/01690969408402123
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The role of semantic transparency in the processing and representation of Dutch compounds

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

26
185
2
5

Year Published

1998
1998
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 240 publications
(218 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
26
185
2
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Zwitserlood (1994) was able to rule out an orthographic explanation, as she failed to find a priming effect for a letter string that was not a compound word constituent (in her orthographic prime condition of Experiment 1, the prime was a compound and the target was a word or a pseudoword; kerstfeest kers). The masked priming study of Longtin, Segui, and Hallé (2003) observed a priming effect of similar magnitude for semantically transparent, opaque and pseudo-derived words (not compounds), whereas no priming was obtained for orthographic controls.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Zwitserlood (1994) was able to rule out an orthographic explanation, as she failed to find a priming effect for a letter string that was not a compound word constituent (in her orthographic prime condition of Experiment 1, the prime was a compound and the target was a word or a pseudoword; kerstfeest kers). The masked priming study of Longtin, Segui, and Hallé (2003) observed a priming effect of similar magnitude for semantically transparent, opaque and pseudo-derived words (not compounds), whereas no priming was obtained for orthographic controls.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…He observed a reliable priming effect in Dutch for transparent compounds (e.g., koe melkfles ¼ cow milkbottle) but not for opaque compounds (e.g., koe melkweg ¼ cow milky way). Zwitserlood (1994) used transparent and opaque Dutch compounds as primes and semantic associates of either the first or second constituent as targets (e.g., transparent: kerkorgel priester ¼ church organ priest; opaque: drankorgel bier ¼ drunkard beer). She found a semantic priming effect for transparent and partly opaque compounds but not for completely opaque compounds.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One might instead expand compound-processing theories that include decomposition of the compound into its constituents (e.g., Libben, 1998;Libben, Gibson, Yoon, & Sandra 2003;Zwitserlood, 1994) by assuming that compounds are stored in the mental lexicon in such a way that compounds that share first constituents and relations are more likely to benefit from the spread of activation from the prime (see Gagné & Spalding, 2006, for a brief discussion). For example, processing the prime snowfort might activate other compounds that include snow as a modifier and that use the MADE OF relation, as compared to those that use the FOR relation.…”
Section: Processing Established Compoundsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Semantically transparent compounds such as snowball fight are usually distinguished from semantically opaque compounds, which are not related to the meaning of their constituting morphemes (e.g. deadline keeping : Sandra 1990;Zwitserlood 1994). …”
Section: Accessing Morphologically Complex Formsmentioning
confidence: 99%